There has been a recent interest in listening effort as a factor to be taken into account in the audiological clinic. However, the
term “listening effort” is poorly determined and needs to be defined before it can be used as a clinical or research tool. One
way of understanding listening effort is in terms of the cognitive resources expended during listening. Cognitive capacity is
finite and thus if cognitive capacity is used up during the act of listening to speech there will be fewer cognitive resources left
to process the content of the message conveyed. We have introduced the term Cognitive Spare Capacity (CSC) to refer to residual cognitive capacity once successful listening has taken place. This extended abstract describes the work we have carried
out to date on measures of CSC for research and clinical use. In the course of this work we have developed tests to assess the
role of memory load, executive function and audiovisual integration in CSC under challenging conditions. When these tests
are fully developed, our aim is that they should allow objective individual assessment of listening effort in cognitive terms. Results to date indicate that under challenging conditions, CSC is an arena for executive processing of temporarily stored information; it is related to individual working memory capacity and can be enhanced by hearing aid signal processing.
REFERENCES(24)
1.
Baddeley A: The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn Sci, 2000; 4(11): 417–23.
Bernstein JGW, Grant KW: Auditory and auditory-visual intelligibility of speech in fluctuating maskers for normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. J Acoust Soc Am, 2009; 125(5): 3358–72.
Foo C, Rudner M, Rönnberg J, Lunner T: Recognition of speech in noise with new hearing instrument compression release settings requires explicit cognitive storage and processing capacity. J Am Acad Audiol, 2007; 18: 553–66.
Fraser S, Gagné J-P, Alepins M, Dubois P: Evaluating the effort expended to understand speech in noise using a dual-task paradigm: The effects of providing visual speech cues. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 2010; 53(1): 18–33.
Gatehouse S, Naylor G, Elberling C: Benefits from hearing aids in relation to the interaction between the user and the environment. Int J Audiol, 2003; 42(Suppl.1): S77–85.
Hagerman B, Kinnefors C: Efficient adaptive methods for measurements of speech reception thresholds in quiet and in noise. Scand Audiol, 1995; 24: 71–77.
Hällgren M, Larsby B, Arlinger S: A Swedish version of the Hearing in Noise Test (HINT) for measurement of speech recognition. Int J Audiol, 2006; 45: 227–37.
Lunner T, Sundewall-Thorén E: Interactions between cognition, compression, and listening conditions: effects on speechin-noise performance in a two-channel hearing aid. J Am Acad Audiol, 2007; 18: 539–52.
Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ et al: The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contribution to complex frontal lobe tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol, 2000; 41: 49–100.
Nilsson M, Soli SD, Sullivan JA: Development of the Hearing In Noise Test for the measurement of speech reception thresholds in quiet and in noise. J Acoust Soc Am, 1994; 95: 1085–99.
Rönnberg J, Rudner M, Foo C, Lunner T: Cognition counts: A working memory system for ease of language understanding (ELU). Int J Audiol, 2008; 47(Suppl. 2): S171–77.
Rudner M, Foo C, Rönnberg J, Lunner T: Cognition and aided speech recognition in noise: specific role for cognitive factors following nine-week experience with adjusted compression settings in hearing aids. Scand J Psychol, 2009; 50: 405–18.
Rudner M, Foo C, Sundewall Thorén E et al: Phonological mismatch and explicit cognitive processing in a sample of 102 hearing aid users. Int J Audiol, 2008; 47 (Suppl. 2): S163–70.
Sarampalis A, Kalluri S, Edwards B et al: Objective measures of listening effort: effects of background noise and noise reduction. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 2009; 52: 1230–40.
Wang D, Kjems U, Pedersen MS et al: Speech intelligibility in background noise with ideal binary time-frequency masking. J Acoust Soc Am, 2009; 125: 2336–47.
We process personal data collected when visiting the website. The function of obtaining information about users and their behavior is carried out by voluntarily entered information in forms and saving cookies in end devices. Data, including cookies, are used to provide services, improve the user experience and to analyze the traffic in accordance with the Privacy policy. Data are also collected and processed by Google Analytics tool (more).
You can change cookies settings in your browser. Restricted use of cookies in the browser configuration may affect some functionalities of the website.